Thursday, December 27, 2007

FINAL SCORES

Hello Everybody,

How’s life going on with you? As fit as a fiddle, right? I hope so.

Here your scores are as follows:


Subject : English Phonology 1
Class : 1D3BI
Scoring Score
Name Tugas Quiz Mid Final Total in Note
20% 20% 25% 35% Letter
Ari Nur H 85 88 90 70 81.6 A PASS
Bayu P 80 80 65 48 65.05 C PASS
Eka Mya W 70 70 100 72 78.2 B PASS
Husnul B 85 90 100 72 85.2 A PASS
Noviana 85 92 90 74 83.8 A PASS
Heriyanto 75 85 60 72 72.2 B PASS
Ria Septiana 70 65 80 77 73.95 B PASS
Siti Khadijah 80 87 100 73 83.95 A PASS
Gustianingsih 80 85 95 68 80.55 A PASS
Hikmah Y 85 80 85 84 83.65 A PASS
Linie 0 0 0 0 0 E FAIL
Anita R 85 90 80 72 80.2 B PASS
Rina Amorsea 85 90 100 69 84.15 A PASS
Taufik Tanaka 80 72 50 43 57.95 D FAIL
I Made 80 65 60 0 44 E FAIL

A wise man says “NO GAIN WITHOUT PAIN”. Be sure what you have got shows what you have done.

Friday, September 21, 2007

The English Phonology

Phonology

Phonology is the study of the sound patterns of language. It is concerned with how sounds are organized in a language. Phonology examines what occurs to speech sounds when they are combined to form a word and how these speech sounds interact with each other. It endeavors to explain what these phonological processes are in terms of formal rules.

  • Phonetics -- What are the sounds? How are they made in the mouth?
  • Phonology -- How do sounds combine? How are they memorized?
Speaker's Mind → Speaker's Mouth → Listener's Ear → Listener's Mind

We will write rules to connect the Mind and Mouth.

Phonemes

The amazing discovery is that people systematically ignore certain properties of sounds. They perceive two different sounds as the same sound. We call the stored versions of speech sounds phonemes. Thus phonemes are the phonetic alphabet of the mind. That is, phonemes are how we mentally represent speech; how we store the sounds of words in our memory.

Though the phonetic alphabet is universal (we can write down the speech sounds actually uttered in any language), the phonemic alphabet varies from language to language. For example, English has no memorized front rounded vowels like German or French, and French has no [θ]. This leads to seemming contradictions when we consider both actual productions of speech sounds as well as their memorized representations. English has no memorized nasal vowels, but English speakers do make nasalized vowels when vowels and nasal consonants come together in speech. The changes between memory and pronuciation are what we will be discovering in this section of the course

Finding Phonemes

How do we find out what's in someone's mind?

How do we figure out how people store the sounds of words in their memories?

One trick that we can use is to look for minimal pairs of words. A minimal pair is a pair of words that have different meanings and which differ in only one sound. Since the difference between the two sounds is meaningful, the words must be stored differently in memory. Since the words differ in only one sound, this difference must be stored in memory. Thus the difference in sounds is significant, and so the two sounds must both be phonemes.

Here is an example from English:

  • [sɪp] and [zɪp]

These two words aredifferent words of English. But they differ only in their initial sound. Therefore, the [s]/[z] difference is significant for English speakers. Therefore both [s] and [z] are stored in the memory. Thus, [s] and [z] are part of the English mental alphabet. We notate elements in the memory by putting them in-between slashes / /. In this case /s/ and /z/ are part of an English speaker's alphabet for memorizing words.

Another example from English:

  • [ræm] and [ræn] and [ræŋ]

These three words are all distinct words of English. Therefore, the speech sounds (in the mouth) [m], [n] and [ŋ] are all significant to the mind. And therefore, English includes the phonemes /m/, /n/ and /ŋ/.

Sometimes it isn't possible to find minimal pairs for all words. But speakers can also tell when a contrast would yield a distinct possible word, even if this is not an actual word.

The phonetic context (or frame) [b_t] can be used to find minimal pairs for many English vowels:

  • [bit] ("beat") /i/
  • [bɪt] ("bit") /ɪ/
  • [bet] ("bait") /e/
  • [bɛt] ("bet") /ɛ/
  • [bæt] ("bat") /æ/
  • [but] ("boot") /u/
  • [bot] ("boat") /o/
  • [bɔt] ("bought") /ɔ/ (You may have [ɑ] here.)
  • [bʌt] ("but") /ʌ/

This minimal set establishes all these vowels as mentally distinct, and therefore phonemic.

Near minimal cases can be found for the other two vowels:

  • [pʊt] ("put") /ʊ/
  • [pɑt] ("pot") /ɑ/

And additional minimal pairs can be constructed to justify all 11 vowels, pair by pair as necessary. Since other minimal pairs can be found ([lʊk] "look" versus [luk] "Luke"), ([kɑt] "cot" versus [kɔt] "caught"), these vowels are also mentally significant, and are therefore phonemes.

Features for Speech Sounds

We will use the articulatory phonetic descriptions to write pronunciation rules.

We will add additional features as necessary.

Rules of pronunciation

The fact that speakers have a mental representation of what they say, and that this can be different from what they actually do when they speak, shows us that speakers do not memorize every aspect of speech sound production. Only the essential (contrastive, phonemic) features are stored in memory. Other features (specifics of pronunciation) are added during speech planning and production. Predictable information about speech is not memorized. Predictable features are added by rules of pronunciation (phonological rules).

A Rule of English

English speakers pronounce vowels either with the velum closed (oral) or with the velum open (nasal). By careful listening or experimental investigation, we can determine that the velum is open during the entire production of the word "man":

  • [mæ̃n]

In contrast, speakers do not have the velum open at all in the production of the word "bat":

  • [bæt]

Most important, however, is that English speakers perceive that both "man" and "bat" have the same vowel. That is, English speakers are ignoring the difference in nasality between the two words. English speakers feel that this difference in nasality is unimportant for recognizing a word in their memory. We can understand this behavior through understanding that speakers memorize vowels without the feature [nasal]. English speakers believe that there are no nasal vowels in English, at least for the purpose of memorizing words.

The reason for this is that in English nasality in vowels is predictable. In English, nasal vowels only occur before nasal consonants. Everywhere else English speakers use oral vowels. Therefore [nasal] is predictable for English vowels, and is governed by a rule of pronunciation:

  • Vowels become nasal when a nasal consonant immediately follows.
  • [vowel] → [nasal] / _ [nasal, consonant]

We can contrast the situations in the Memory and in the Mouth:

Memory:
/æ/
Mouth: [æ̃]
only before nasal consonants

[æ]
Everywhere else (Elsewhere)

We call the mental representation a phoneme, and we call the distinct pronunciations allophones.

The predictable aspects of pronunciation (here [nasal] in vowels) are added by the rules in the phonology of the language. The rules of pronunciation determine the variants in speech sounds. This particular rule makes one sound (the vowel) more similar to an adjacent sound (the following nasal consonant), by making the vowel [nasal]. Rules that make sounds more similar are called assimilation rules. Rules that make sounds less similar are called dissimilation rules. Assimilation rules are much more common than dissimilation rules.

Language variation

Which features are predictable varies from language to language. In French speakers must memorize [+nasal] for vowels because in French this is important for the meaning of the word. That is, French has minimal pairs for nasality in vowels.

Feature redundancies

Within a single sound some of the aspects of speech sound production (features) may be predictable from the other features. Here are some examples from English:

  • All nasals are voiced: [nasal] → [voiced]
  • all high back vowels are round: [vowel, high, back] → [rounded]
  • all front vowels are not round: [vowel, front] → [unrounded]

These particular rules are rules of English. Other languages may or may not have these rules. So, for instance French has frount rounded vowels, but no high back unround vowels. Russian has the reverse: high back unround vowels but no front round vowels. Turkish has both front round vowels and high back unround vowels.

Changes due to context

All languages impose certain restrictions on the sequences of sounds in the language. Some languages like to alternate consonants and vowels. These language do not allow sequences of consonants nor sequences of vowels.

There are two possible responses a language can make to an unwanted sequence. One is to change one of the sounds, through a rule. This is what we observed with English nasal vowels. Oral vowels are not allowed to be followed by nasal consonants, so the vowel is changed to be nasal.

The other possible response is to simply ban the sequence from words as they are stored in memory. In English there is a general ban on words beginning with *[tl] and *[dl], even though words starting with [pl], [bl], [kl] and [gl] are fine. But there is no general rule to repair these bad sequences.

Neutralization

We have seen the case of one mental sound having two mouth pronunciations, repeated here:

Memory:
/æ/
Mouth: [æ̃]
only before nasal consonants

[æ]
Everywhere else (Elsewhere)

It is also possible to have the situation where sound that are memorized differently are nevertheless pronounced identically under certain circumstances. Consider the pronuncation of the vowels in these two words:

  • [tɛləgræf] "telegraph"
  • [təlɛgrəfi] "telegraphy"

But since both of these words involve the same morpheme, meaning "telegraph", this morpheme must have the same memorized representation, namely,

  • /tɛlɛgræf/

Therefore the changes in pronunciation are insignificant for memory here, and must be due to a rule of pronunciation. The rules is very simple, unstressed vowels reduce to schwa in English.

  • [vowel, unstressed] → [ə]
  • [vowel, unstressed] → [mid, central, unrounded, plain]

This means that in this case we have two different sounds as far as the mind goes, and yet when unstressed, only one pronunciation:

Mind: /æ/
/ɛ/
Mouth:
[ə]
when unstressed

The pronunciation of the English plural

The plural marker in English has several different pronuncations:

  • [∅] (zero): [dir], [ʃip], [fɪʃ], etc.
  • [ə̃n]: [ɑksə̃n], etc.
  • [s]: [kæts], [pɑts], [sæks], etc.
  • [z]: [dɑgz], [rɪbz], [bænz], etc.
  • [əz]: [bʊʃəz], [rozəz], etc.

The choice of zero or [ə̃n] is handled in the Morphology, because it is not predictable from the speech sounds. The choice of [s], [z] or [əz] is handled in the Phonology, because it is predictable from the speech sounds.

So we need to figure out what conditions the appearance of the various pronunciations of the plural. Where do we get which pronunciations?

Pronunciations:    [s]        [z]       [əz]
Examples: cats dads bushes
beliefs sleeves roses
cups cabs kisses
tacks tags churches
paths wreathes judges
dams
cans
songs
fears
peels
bees
bays
boos
bows
boys

We want to predict the pronunciations, so we need to pick one of the pronunciations to be stored in memory. We should pick the least predictable one to store in memory, here [z]. Therefore, the memorized representation for the plural morpheme is /z/.

Now we need to write rules to get the other pronunciations.

What is similar about all the stems in the first column, the ones that take [s] in the plural? They are end in voiceless sounds! So the rule for these must be:

/z/ becomes [s] when it comes after a voiceless sound

But we can do better than this. What is the difference between /z/ and [s]? /z/ is [voiced] and [s] is [voiceless]. So we can write the rule as:

/z/ becomes [voiceless] when it comes after a [voiceless] sound

/z/ → [voiceless] / [voiceless] _

Thus, this is a rule of assimilation.

What is the characteristic shared by the words in the third column, where they say [əz] for the plural? All of these words end in sibilant (see FRH p. 300) sounds. So we can write the rule:

Insert [ə] in between a sibilant and /z/

Again, we can ask if we can do better. Is /z/ sibilant? Yes! So we can write the rule:

Insert [ə] inbetween two sibilants

∅ → [ə] / [sibilant] __ [sibilant]

The use of the zero here for "no sound" or "nothing" is similar to the concept of zero morphemes. For insertion, ∅ → something; for deletion, something → ∅.

Doing Phonology Problems

The basic steps in doing phonology problems are:
  1. Look for minimal pairs (If yes, then both sounds are phonemes)
  2. Look for alternations in the pronunciation of one morpheme
  3. List the environments for the different pronunciations
  4. Find the part of the environment that predicts the change in pronunciation
  5. Make a mind and mouth diagram
  6. Write the rule (in words, then in symbols)
  7. Check to see that the rule works

Exercise 7.3: Korean (part 1)

Question

Consider the distribution of [r] and [l] in Korean in the following words. Some additional details of Korean pronunciation which are not indicated in the book are given here.

rubi   'ruby'
kiri 'road'
saram 'person'
irɯmi 'name'
radio 'radio'

mul 'water'
pal 'big'
səul 'Seoul'
ilgop 'seven'
ipalsa 'barber'

Are [r] and [l] allophones of one or two phonemes? (That is, are these sounds stored differently in the memory of Korean speakers?) State your reasons, and give the rule to derive the surface phones if you conclude that they are allophonic.

Answer

  1. Look for minimal pairs

    There are none. Therefore, probably allophones of one phoneme. Two sounds in the mouth, one sound in the mind.

  2. Look for morphemes with changing pronunciation

    There are none.

  3. List the environments for the sounds

    (Zoom in on the things of interest. Get rid of extraneous information.) Work from the things closest to the sounds of interest.

    [r]
    [l]
    sounds immediately before sounds immediately after sounds immediately before sounds immediately after
    # (beginning of word), i, a u, i, a, ɯ u, a, i # (end of word), k, s
  4. Find the part of the environment that predicts the change in pronunciation

    Does the "before" environment predict which sound? NO! The sounds [i] and [a] occur before both [r] and [l]. Therefore we cannot predict [r] versus [l] on the basis of the immediately preceding sound. In the case of [i] or [a] both [r] and [l] can occur.

    Does the "after" environment predict [r] versus [l]? YES! The "after" environments do not overlap!

    What is the difference between the two environments? [r] occurs only when a vowel follows; [l] occurs when either a consonant follows or when nothing follows (i.e. at the end of the word).

    Which is the simpler one to predict? [r], "vowel" is simpler than "consonant or nothing". Therefore the [r] is predictable, and /l/ (the unpredictable one) must be the phoneme.

  5. Make a mind and mouth diagram
    Mind:                        /l/
    Mouth: [r] / _ [vowel] [l] / Elsewhere
  6. Write the rule (in words, then in symbols)
    Koreans pronounce /l/ as [r] when a vowel immediately follows.

    /l/ → [r] / __ [vowel]

    Can we do better? What's the difference between [l] and [r]? [l] is [lateral]; [r] is [alveolar].

    [lateral] → [alveolar] / __ [vowel]
  7. Check to see that the rule works

Finnish (From an earlier edition of the textbook)

Consider the following data from Finnish:

a. [ku:zi] 'six'
b. [kudot] 'failures'
c. [kate] 'cover'
d. [katot] 'roofs'
e. [kade] 'envious'
f. [ku:si] 'six'
g. [li:sa] 'Lisa'
h. [maton] 'of a worm'
i. [madon] 'of a rug'
j. [ratas] 'wheel'
k. [li:za] 'Lisa'
l. [radan] 'of a track'
  1. Do [s] and [z] represent different phonemes? (Do Finnish speakers use both /s/ and /z/ to store words in their memories?)

    1. Look for minimal pairs (If yes, then both sounds are phonemes)

      a. [ku:zi] and f. [ku:si] differ minimally in pronunciation in the right way: a. has [z] and f. has [s].

      BUT they both have the same meaning: 'six'. Because the difference in pronunciation does NOT cause a change in meaning, this is not a minimal pair. Therefore these two words do not establish a mental memorized distinction between [s] and [z].

    2. Look for alternations in the pronunciation of one morpheme

      We already found one: a. [ku:zi] or f. [ku:si] 'six'. And there's another: g. [li:sa] or k. [li:za] 'Lisa'.

    3. List the environments for the different pronunciations

      [s]
      [z]
      sounds immediately before sounds immediately after sounds immediately before sounds immediately after
      u:, i:, a i, a, # (end of word) u:, i: i, a

    4. Find the part of the environment that predicts the change in pronunciation.

      Can we predict [s] versus [z] from the "before" environment? NO! The "before" environments overlap, both [s] and [z] can occur when the immediately preceding sound is [u:] or [i:]. Therefore, knowing the immediately preceding sound won't predict which of [s] or [z] we get.

      Can we predict [s] versus [z] from the "after" environment? NO! The "after" environments overlap, both [s] and [z] can occur when the immediately following sound is [i] or [a]. Therefore, knowing the immediately following sound won't predict which of [s] or [z] we get.

      Moreover, in the cases of the false minimal pairs,
      a. [ku:zi] and f. [ku:si] 'six'
      g. [li:sa] and k. [li:za] 'Lisa'
      the environments are exactly the same.
      (They look like minimal pairs, but they aren't because the change in pronunciation doesn't cause a change in meaning.) Since the environments are exactly the same in these alternative pronunciations of 'Lisa' and 'six', nothing in the speech sounds themselves can predict which one of [s] or [z] will be used. This case is not phonologically predictable. It might be the case that looking at social or dialect factors would tell us how Finnish speakers choose between [s] and [z]. At this point all we know is that they don't make the choice between [s] and [z] based on the surrounding sounds.

      However, we can notice that [s] occurs in more environments than [z] does. For example, j. [ratas] 'whell' has an [s] at the end of the word. We don't see any words ending in [z]. So it would be best to memorize the sound as /s/, and then sometimes pronounce /s/ as [z]. For this case we just can't predict exactly when /s/ is pronounced as [z].

    5. Make a mind and mouth diagram
      Mind:        /s/
      Mouth: [s] [z]
      Overlapping environments--unpredictable
    6. Write the rule (first in words, then in symbols)
      /s/ becomes [z] unpredictably

      /s/ → [z] unpredictably

      /s/ → [voiced] unpredictably

      [s] and [z] are in free variation

  2. Do [d] and [t] represent different phonemes? (Do Finnish speakers use both /d/ and /t/ to store words in their memories?)

    1. Look for minimal pairs (If yes, then both sounds are phonemes)

      There are minimal pairs:

      • h. [madon] 'of a worm'
      • i. [maton] 'of a rug'

      [madon] and [maton] differ minimally in their pronounciation, AND this change in pronunciation goes along with a change in meaning: [madon] means 'of a worm' and [maton] means 'of a rug'. Because we found minimal pairs, both sounds are phonemes.

    2. Look for alternations in the pronunciation of one morpheme

      There are no alternations for [t] and [d].

    3. List the environments for the different pronunciations

      Because they are both phonemes (both used in memory) and there are no alternations, we do not need to calculate the environment, because there is nothing to predict.

      In Finnish, /t/ is pronounced as [t] and /d/ is pronounced as [d].

    4. Find the part of the environment that predicts the change in pronunciation

      There is nothing to predict here. Both /t/ and /d/ are phonemes, and they maintain separate pronunciations (there are no alternations).

    5. Make a mind and mouth diagram
      Mind:      /t/          /d/
      Mouth: [t] [d]
    6. Write the rule

      No rule here, they're both phonemes with no alternations.


Exercise 7.12: Hebrew

Consider the phonetic forms of Hebrew words:

[v]-[b]
bika 'lamented'
mugbal 'limited'
ʃavar 'broke masculine'
ʃavra 'broke feminine'
ʔikev 'delayed'
bara 'created'

[f]-[p]
litef 'stroked'
sefer 'book'
sataf 'washed'
para 'cow'
mitpaxat 'handkerchief'
haʔalpim 'the Alps'

Assume that these words and their phonetic sequences are representative of what may occur in Hebrew. In your answers below, consider classes of sounds rather than individual sounds.

  1. Are [b] and [v] allophones of one phoneme? (Are [b] and [v] memorized as separate sounds, or are they stored in memory as the same sound?) (Hint: Are they in complementary distribution? Do the environments overlap? Can you predict which will occur?)

    1. Look for minimal pairs (If yes, then both sounds are phonemes)

      There are no minimal pairs for [b] versus [v].

    2. Look for alternations in the pronunciation of one morpheme

      The morpheme for 'broke' shows different pronunciations for the masculine and the feminine: [ʃavar] 'broke masculine' and [ʃavra] 'broke feminine'. But these two forms do not illustrate a [b]-[v] change. Therefore they don't immediately let us figure out what causes either [b] or [v] to occur. So we have to go on to listing the environments.

    3. List the environments for the different pronunciations

      [b]
      [v]
      sounds immediately before sounds immediately after sounds immediately before sounds immediately after
      # (beginning of word), g i, a a, e a, r, # (end of word)

    4. Find the part of the environment that predicts the change in pronunciation

      The "after" environments overlap. The sound [a] can come immediately after both [v] and [b]. Therefore we cannot predict which of [v] or [b] would occur just given the information about what sound comes immediately after.

      The "before" environments do not overlap. Therefore the before environment is sufficient to predict which of [b] or [v] will occur. Therefore for speakers of Hebrew, [b] and [v] are stored as one sound.

      But which sound do you store in memory? We figure this out by asking which environment is harder to predict. In this data, [v] only occurs after vowels. [b] never occurs after vowels. [b] only occurs after consonants, or at the beginning of the word. The environment "after vowels" is simpler than "after consonants or at the beginning of the word", so the environment for [b] is harder to predict. Therefore, for Hebrew speakers [b] and [v] are different pronunciations of the memorized sound /b/.

    5. Make a mind and mouth diagram
      Mind:                      /b/
      Mouth: [b] / Elsewhere [v] / [vowel] _
    6. Write the rule (in words, then in symbols)
      /b/ becomes [v] when there is a vowel immediately before it

      /b/ → [v] / [vowel] __

      [voiced oral bilabial stop] → [fricative] / [vowel] __
  2. Does the same rule that describes the distribution of [b] and [v] apply to [p] and [f]?

    What do we want to compare? [b] with [p] or [b] with [f]? We want to compare stops with stops and fricatives with fricatives. Therefore, we want to compare [b] with [p]. Therefore, we are considering whether the rule should be extended to include:

    /p/ --> [f] / [-consonantal] __

    How would we know? The rule says that we get [f] if and only if it is preceded by a vowel. Therefore, there can be two kinds of counterexamples:

    • [f] preceded by something other than a vowel (consonant or at the beginning of the word -- the undergeneration case)
    • [p] preceded by a vowel (the overgeneration case)

    So we look. All [f]'s are immediately preceded by vowels. No [p] is immediately preceded by a vowel. Therefore the rule works here too.

    Now we need to write one rule that covers both cases. What characteristics do [p] and [b] share? They're both labial stops. Therefore the rule must be:

    A labial oral stop becomes a fricative when a vowel comes immediately before it

    [labial oral stop] → [fricative] / [vowel] __
  3. Here is a word with one phone missing. A blank appears in place of the missing sound: hid__ik.

    What determines the choice of [b, p] versus [v, f]? The sound immediately before. What sound is immediately before the blank? [d]. What kind of sound is [d]? A consonant. So fricatives cannot appear here, because fricatives only appear when there is a vowel immediately before them.

    So "(1) [b] but not [v] could occur in the empty slot" is the only correct statement.

  4. Which one of the following statements is correct about the incomplete word __ana?

    No vowel preceding, so fricatives cannot occur in this position.

    So "(2) [p] but not [f] could occur in the empty slot" is the only correct statement.

  5. Now consider the following possible words (in phonetic transcription) (Assume that all words have different meanings.)
    laval
    surva
    labal
    palar
    falu
    razif

    If these words actually occured in Hebrew would they:

    1. Force you to revise the conclusions about the distribution of labial stops and fricatives you reached on the basis of the first group of words given above.
    2. Support your original conslusions?
    3. Neither support not disprove your original conclusions.

    Let's check to see if we need to rethink anything.

    1. Look for minimal pairs (If yes, then both sounds are phonemes)

      The new data introduces a minimal pair: [laval] versus [labal]. This would lead us to conclude that the distinction between [b] and [v] is significant, because it goes along with a change in meaning. Then we would conclude that Hebrew speakers memorize both [b] and [v], giving two phonemes /b/ and /v/. This is clearly a different conclusion than what we came up with, so the right answer is (1), we would be forced to revise our conclusions.

Links for English Phonology

Here, you all guys can make your learning activities FUN, getting better & comfortable. Just check these out!

English Phonology, by Ted Power (ELT)

The International Phonetic Alphabet

The Human Vocal Tract

Phonology, Phonics, and English Spelling

Learning the IPA for English

Applied English Phonology

ENGLISH PHONOLOGY

Tuesday, September 18, 2007

Parts of Human Vocal Tract


Remarks
1. Alveolar Ridge
2. Hard Palate
3. Velum (Soft Palate)
4. Uvula
5. Epiglottis
6. Larynx
7. Glottis
8. Vocal Folds
9. Tongue
10. Teeth
11. Lips
12. Nostril / Nasal Cavity

by. syafii_smk3komet@yahoo.com